class: center, middle, inverse, title-slide # STA 360/602L: Module 3.6 ## Noninformative and improper priors ### Dr. Olanrewaju Michael Akande --- ## Noninformative and improper priors - Generally, we must specify both `\(\mu_0\)` and `\(\tau_0\)` to do inference. -- - When prior distributions have no population basis, that is, there is no justification of the prior as "prior data", prior distributions can be difficult to construct. -- - To that end, there is often the desire to construct .hlight[noninformative priors], with the rationale being _"to let the data speak for themselves"_. -- - For example, we could instead assume a uniform prior on `\(\mu\)` that is constant over the real line, i.e., `\(\pi(\mu) \propto 1\)` `\(\Rightarrow\)` all values on the real line are equally likely apriori. -- - Clearly, this is not a valid pdf since it will not integrate to 1 over the real line. Such priors are known as .hlight[improper priors]. -- - An improper prior can still be very useful, we just need to ensure it results in a .hlight[proper posterior]. --- ## Jeffreys' prior - Question: is there a prior pdf (for a given model) that would be universally accepted as a noninformative prior? -- - Laplace proposed the uniform distribution. This proposal lacks invariance under monotone transformations of the parameter. -- - For example, a uniform prior on the binomial proportion parameter `\(\theta\)` is not the same as a uniform prior on the odds parameter `\(\phi = \dfrac{\theta}{1-\theta}\)`. -- - A more acceptable approach was introduced by Jeffreys. For single parameter models, the .hlight[Jeffreys' prior] defines a noninformative prior density of a parameter `\(\theta\)` as .block[ .small[ `$$\pi(\theta) \propto \sqrt{\mathcal{I}(\theta)}$$` ] ] where `\(\mathcal{I}(\theta)\)` is the .hlight[Fisher information] for `\(\theta\)`. --- ## Jeffreys' prior - The Fisher information gives a way to measure the amount of information a random variable `\(Y\)` carries about an unknown parameter `\(\theta\)` of a distribution that describes `\(Y\)`. -- - Formally, `\(\mathcal{I}(\theta)\)` is defined as .block[ .small[ `$$\mathcal{I}(\theta) = \mathbb{E} \left[ \left( \dfrac{\partial}{\partial \theta} \textrm{log } p(y | \theta) \right)^2 \bigg| \theta \right] = \int_\mathcal{Y} \left( \dfrac{\partial}{\partial \theta} \textrm{log } p(y | \theta) \right)^2 p(y | \theta) dy.$$` ] ] -- - Alternatively, .block[ .small[ `$$\mathcal{I}(\theta) = - \mathbb{E} \left[ \dfrac{\partial^2}{\partial^2 \theta} \textrm{log } p(y | \theta) \bigg| \theta \right] = - \int_\mathcal{Y} \left( \dfrac{\partial^2}{\partial^2 \theta} \textrm{log } p(y | \theta) \right) p(y | \theta) dy.$$` ] ] -- - Turns out that the Jeffreys' prior for `\(\mu\)` under the normal model, when `\(\sigma^2\)` is known, is .block[ .small[ `$$\pi(\mu) \propto 1,$$` ] ] the uniform prior over the real line. Let's derive this on the board. --- ## Inference for mean, conditional on variance using Jeffreys' prior - Recall that for `\(\sigma^2\)` known, the normal likelihood simplifies to .block[ .small[ `$$\propto \ \textrm{exp}\left\{-\dfrac{1}{2} \tau n(\mu - \bar{y})^2 \right\},$$` ] ] ignoring everything else that does not depend on `\(\mu\)`. - With the Jeffreys' prior `\(\pi(\mu) \propto 1\)`, can we derive the posterior distribution? --- ## Inference for mean, conditional on variance using Jeffreys' prior - Posterior: .block[ .small[ $$ `\begin{split} \pi(\mu|Y,\tau) \ & \propto \ \textrm{exp}\left\{-\dfrac{1}{2} \tau n(\mu - \bar{y})^2 \right\} \pi(\mu)\\ & \propto \ \textrm{exp}\left\{-\dfrac{1}{2} \tau n(\mu - \bar{y})^2 \right\}.\\ \end{split}` $$ ] ] -- - This is the kernel of a normal distribution with + mean `\(\bar{y}\)`, and + precision `\(n\tau\)` or variance `\(\dfrac{1}{n\tau} = \dfrac{\sigma^2}{n}\)`. -- - Written differently, we have `\(\mu|Y,\sigma^2 \sim \mathcal{N}(\bar{y},\dfrac{\sigma^2}{n})\)` -- - <div class="question"> This should look familiar to you. Does it? </div> --- ## Improper prior - Let's be very objective with the prior selection. In fact, let's be extreme! -- + If we let the normal variance `\(\rightarrow \infty\)` then our prior on `\(\mu\)` is `\(\propto 1\)` (recall the Jeffreys' prior on `\(\mu\)` for known `\(\sigma^2\)`). -- + If we let the gamma variance get very large (e.g., `\(a,b \rightarrow 0\)`), then the prior on `\(\sigma^2\)` is `\(\propto \dfrac{1}{\sigma^2}\)`. -- - `\(\pi(\mu,\sigma^2) \propto \dfrac{1}{\sigma^2}\)` is improper (does not integrate to 1) but does lead to a proper posterior distribution that yields inferences similar to frequentist ones. -- - For that choice, we have .block[ .small[ $$ `\begin{split} \mu|Y,\tau & \sim \mathcal{N}\left(\bar{y},\dfrac{1}{n \tau}\right)\\ \tau | Y & \sim \textrm{Gamma}\left(\dfrac{n-1}{2}, \dfrac{(n-1)s^2}{2}\right)\\ \end{split}` $$ ] ] --- ## Analysis with noninformative priors - Recall the Pygmalion data: + Accelerated group (A): 20, 10, 19, 15, 9, 18. + No growth group (N): 3, 2, 6, 10, 11, 5. -- - Summary statistics: + `\(\bar{y}_A = 15.2\)`; `\(s_A = 4.71\)`. + `\(\bar{y}_N = 6.2\)`; `\(s_N = 3.65\)`. -- - So our joint posterior is .block[ .small[ $$ `\begin{split} \mu_A | Y_A, \tau_A & \sim \ \mathcal{N}\left(\bar{y}_A,\dfrac{1}{n_A \tau_A}\right) = \mathcal{N}\left(15.2, \dfrac{1}{6\tau_A} \right)\\ \tau_A | Y_A & \sim \textrm{Gamma}\left(\dfrac{n_A-1}{2}, \dfrac{(n_A-1)s^2_A}{2}\right) = \textrm{Gamma}\left(\dfrac{6-1}{2}, \dfrac{(6-1)(22.17)}{2}\right)\\ \mu_N | Y_N, \tau_N & \sim \ \mathcal{N}\left(\bar{y}_N,\dfrac{1}{n_N \tau_N}\right) = \mathcal{N}\left(6.2, \dfrac{1}{6\tau_N} \right)\\ \tau_N | Y_N & \sim \textrm{Gamma}\left(\dfrac{n_N-1}{2}, \dfrac{(n_N-1)s^2_A}{2}\right) = \textrm{Gamma}\left(\dfrac{6-1}{2}, \dfrac{(6-1)(13.37)}{2}\right)\\ \end{split}` $$ ] ] --- ## Monte Carlo sampling It is easy to sample from these posteriors: ```r aA <- (6-1)/2 aN <- (6-1)/2 bA <- (6-1)*22.17/2 bN <- (6-1)*13.37/2 muA <- 15.2 muN <- 6.2 tauA_postsample_impr <- rgamma(10000,aA,bA) thetaA_postsample_impr <- rnorm(10000,muA,sqrt(1/(6*tauA_postsample_impr))) tauN_postsample_impr <- rgamma(10000,aN,bN) thetaN_postsample_impr <- rnorm(10000,muN,sqrt(1/(6*tauN_postsample_impr))) sigma2A_postsample_impr <- 1/tauA_postsample_impr sigma2N_postsample_impr <- 1/tauN_postsample_impr ``` --- ## Monte Carlo sampling - Is the average improvement for the accelerated group larger than that for the no growth group? + What is `\(\Pr[\mu_A > \mu_N | Y_A, Y_N)\)`? ```r mean(thetaA_postsample_impr > thetaN_postsample_impr) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.9955 ``` -- - Is the variance of improvement scores for the accelerated group larger than that for the no growth group? + What is `\(\Pr[\sigma^2_A > \sigma^2_N | Y_A, Y_N)\)`? ```r mean(sigma2A_postsample_impr > sigma2N_postsample_impr) ``` ``` ## [1] 0.7007 ``` -- - <div class="question"> How does the new choice of prior affect our conclusions? </div> --- ## Recall the job training data - Data: + No training group (N): sample size `\(n_N = 429\)`. + Training group (T): sample size `\(n_A = 185\)`. -- - Summary statistics for change in annual earnings: + `\(\bar{y}_N = 1364.93\)`; `\(s_N = 7460.05\)` + `\(\bar{y}_T = 4253.57\)`; `\(s_T = 8926.99\)` -- - <div class="question"> Using the same approach we used for the Pygmalion data, answer the questions of interest. </div> --- class: center, middle # What's next? ### Move on to the readings for the next module!